10 interesting stories served every morning and every evening.




1 513 shares, 41 trendiness

Egypt declared malaria-free by World Health Organization

Egypt has been cer­ti­fied malaria-free by the World Health Organization (WHO) - an achieve­ment hailed by the UN pub­lic health agency as truly his­toric”.

Malaria is as old as Egyptian civ­i­liza­tion it­self, but the dis­ease that plagued pharaohs now be­longs to its his­tory,” said WHO chief Tedros Adhanom Ghebreyesus.

Egyptian au­thor­i­ties launched their first ef­forts to stamp out the deadly mos­quito-borne in­fec­tious dis­ease nearly 100 years.

Certification is granted when a coun­try proves that the trans­mis­sion chain is in­ter­rupted for at least the pre­vi­ous three con­sec­u­tive years. Malaria kills at least 600,000 peo­ple every year, nearly all of them in Africa.

...

Read the original on www.bbc.com »

2 266 shares, 28 trendiness

Please do not write below this line

Please do not write be­low this line

I have been vexed for some time by the re­quest at the bot­tom of

each let­ter that I am not to write be­low the line.

I emailed TVL/BBC on 5 November 2006 to find out why:

I’ve had a let­ter from TV Licencing and I’m in­ter­ested in the

state­ment at the bot­tom of the page. It says: Please do not write be­low this

line’. I would like to know why the let­ter re­quests this. The line re­ferred to

is about half an inch from the bot­tom edge of the let­ter.

What will hap­pen if I write there? How would you know? I am not

asked to re­turn the let­ter, so why the re­quest?

Seven weeks later, on 27 December 2006, I re­ceive

this from one Kelly Wright:

Thank you for con­tact­ing us. Unfortunately I am un­able to deal

with your re­quest, as you have not pro­vided your ad­dress and li­cence num­ber. If

you have moved ad­dress I will need both your new and old ad­dress. Once I have

this in­for­ma­tion I will ac­tion your re­quest and send you the ap­pro­pri­ate form

or con­fir­ma­tion.

I do not

have a li­cence. The let­ter was sent to me as part of TVLs rou­tine mail-out. It

was not so­licited by me. Copies of these let­ters are com­monly re­pro­duced on the

in­ter­net [example].

You will see that these let­ters say Please do not

write be­low this line”. So did the one sent to me. Please ex­plain to me why I

am not al­lowed to write be­low the line.

Evidently, my ques­tion was too tax­ing for Ms

Wright, as the next re­sponse came from Ruairi Mcclean, on 3 January

2007:

The rea­son you would re­ceive such let­ters is be­cause we would have

no record of a TV li­cence at your ad­dress.

The rea­son you can­not write be­low the line is be­cause the let­ters

go through a OCR ma­chine, and any­thing be­low the line is re­jected.

OCR is an ab­bre­vi­a­tion for optical char­ac­ter

recog­ni­tion”, soft­ware that scans doc­u­ments for edit­ing on a com­puter.

This ex­pla­na­tion sur­prised me. I did not

un­der­stand why, hav­ing sent me a let­ter, TVL wanted it back to scan and edit;

why not scan it be­fore send­ing it to me? I re­sponded on 27 January

2007:

Thank you

for your re­ply. You say that I can­not write be­low the line be­cause the let­ter

will go through a OCR ma­chine, and any­thing be­low the line will be re­jected.

I have two fur­ther ques­tions:

i) I take it from your re­ply that

a TV of­fi­cer is plan­ning to col­lect the let­ter back off me in or­der to scan it.

Please tell me what pur­pose this serves.

ii) Anything I write above the line would also be

re­jected by the OCR. Why am I al­lowed to write above the line, if I am not

al­lowed to write in the nar­row strip be­neath it?

On 30 January 2007, I re­ceived this re­ply from Cas

Scott:

A

Licensing of­fi­cer may call at your prop­erty not to col­lect the let­ters but to

check that you are not watch­ing a TV.

You may write above the line but as we ad­vised you

pre­vi­ously any­thing writ­ten be­low the line when they go through the OCR ma­chine

they will be re­jected.

If you would like to con­firm your ad­dress I can up date our

records to ad­vise no Television is be­ing watched.

Thank you for con­firm­ing that I may write above the line.

Please ex­plain why, hav­ing sent me the

let­ter, you want it back for scan­ning. Also, please ex­plain how I am to get the

let­ter to you.

Without your ad­dress we are un­able to amend our records to show

that you are not us­ing TV equip­ment.

To re­turn an en­quiry let­ter to TV li­cens­ing,

sim­ply re­turn it.

I don’t seem to be get­ting a straight

an­swer.

Thank you for your re­ply. The pur­pose of my query is not to ask

why you want my ad­dress.

The in­for­ma­tion that I am seek­ing is why you want the let­ter back

for scan­ning. There was noth­ing on the let­ter that said I had to re­turn it.

Please note that I

am not Miss Scott.

The in­for­ma­tion about re­turn­ing the let­ter was not on the let­ter

it­self but on the en­ve­lope.

The only

rea­son we ask you to re­turn the let­ter is to help us up­date our records,

how­ever if you could pro­vide us with your ad­dress we can up­date our records

with­out you re­turn­ing the let­ter.

Having kept all my TVL/BBC en­velopes, I ex­am­ined

them to see whether any dis­played an in­struc­tion that I was to re­turn the

let­ter. None did. There was a re­turn ad­dress but only was for un­de­liv­ered

let­ters. I re­sist the temp­ta­tion to pur­sue this point.

Thank you for your re­ply.

What I still do not un­der­stand is why you would

wish to OCR my let­ter in or­der to up­date the records.

Obviously, the num­ber be­low the line must be very

im­por­tant. Please could you ex­plain its pur­pose.

I

apol­o­gise that it has not been made clear to you. An OCR stands for a Optical

Character System. This ma­chine en­ables us to deal/​process with large vol­umes of

in­for­ma­tion in a rel­a­tively short space of time. The OCR ma­chine reads the

in­for­ma­tion be­low the line and up­dates the cor­re­spond­ing records on our

com­puter sys­tem many times faster than if man­u­ally processed.

If the in­for­ma­tion

be­low the line is ob­scured in any­way the OCR ma­chine will be un­able to read the

in­for­ma­tion ef­fec­tively. The num­ber be­low the line is a unique num­ber that

re­lates to the spe­cific prop­erty that the let­ter has been sent to. Once this

num­ber is read by the OCR ma­chine it will au­to­mat­i­cally up­date the com­puter

records that re­late to that let­ter/​prop­erty/​li­cence/​ap­pli­ca­tion.

...

Read the original on www.bbctvlicence.com »

3 254 shares, 18 trendiness

maoserr/epublifier: Converts some webnovels to epub format

A tool that al­lows you to ex­tract a list of html pages from a web­site and com­pile them into an ePub book to be im­ported into your eReader of choice.

For ad­vanced users who can write javascript, you can add ad­di­tional parser de­f­i­n­i­tion to cus­tomize pars­ing of any site.

Check out the wiki for us­age.

Custom sites with UL/OL el­e­ments as table of con­tent, or regex on Link text, or use query se­lec­tor

Custom web app with pre­de­fined Title (header) el­e­ment and Next but­ton (clickable)

...

Read the original on github.com »

4 228 shares, 20 trendiness

Dracula author Bram Stoker's lost story unearthed after 134 years

An am­a­teur his­to­rian has dis­cov­ered a long-lost short story by Bram Stoker, pub­lished just seven years be­fore his leg­endary gothic novel Dracula. Brian Cleary stum­bled upon the 134-year-old ghostly tale while brows­ing the archives of the National Library of Ireland.Gibbet Hill was orig­i­nally pub­lished in a Dublin news­pa­per in 1890 - when the Irishman started work­ing on Dracula - but has been un­doc­u­mented ever since.Stoker bi­og­ra­pher Paul Murray says the story sheds light on his de­vel­op­ment as an au­thor and was a sig­nif­i­cant station on his route to pub­lish­ing Dracula”.

The ghostly story tells the tale of a sailor mur­dered by three crim­i­nals whose bod­ies were strung up on a hang­ing gal­lows as a warn­ing to pass­ing trav­ellers. It is set in Gibbet Hill in Surrey, a lo­ca­tion also ref­er­enced in Charles Dickens’ 1839 novel Nicholas Nickleby.Mr Cleary made the dis­cov­ery af­ter tak­ing time off work fol­low­ing a sud­den on­set of hear­ing loss in 2021 - dur­ing which pe­riod he would pass the time at the na­tional li­brary in Stoker’s na­tive Dublin.In October 2023, the Stoker fan came across an un­fa­mil­iar ti­tle in an 1890 Christmas sup­ple­ment of the Daily Express Dublin Edition.Mr Clearly told the AFP news agency: I read the words Gibbet Hill and I knew that was­n’t a Bram Stoker story that I had ever heard of in any of the bi­ogra­phies or bib­li­ogra­phies.“”I sat look­ing at the screen won­der­ing, am I the only liv­ing per­son who had read it?”He said of the mo­ment he made the dis­cov­ery: What on earth do I do with it?”The li­brary’s di­rec­tor Audrey Whitty said Mr Cleary called her and said: I’ve found some­thing ex­tra­or­di­nary in your news­pa­per archives - you won’t be­lieve it.“She added that his astonishing am­a­teur de­tec­tive work” was a tes­ta­ment to the li­brary’s archives.“There are truly world-im­por­tant dis­cov­er­ies wait­ing to be found”, she said.

After his ini­tial sleuthing, Mr Cleary con­tacted bi­og­ra­pher Paul Murray - who con­firmed there had been no trace of the story for over a cen­tury. He said 1890 was when he was a young writer and made his first notes for Dracula.“It’s a clas­sic Stoker story, the strug­gle be­tween good and evil, evil which crops up in ex­otic and un­ex­plained ways,” he added.Gib­bet Hill is be­ing pub­lished along­side art­work by the Irish artist Paul McKinley by the Rotunda Foundation - the fundrais­ing arm of Dublin’s Rotunda Hospital for which Mr Cleary worked.All pro­ceeds will go to the newly formed Charlotte Stoker Fund - named af­ter Bram Stoker’s mother who was a hear­ing loss cam­paigner - to fund re­search on in­fant hear­ing loss.The dis­cov­ery is also be­ing high­lighted in the city’s Bram Stoker fes­ti­val later this month.

...

Read the original on www.bbc.com »

5 183 shares, 26 trendiness

T-Mobile, AT&T oppose unlocking rule, claim locked phones are good for users

Carriers fight plan to re­quire un­lock­ing of phones 60 days af­ter ac­ti­va­tion.

T-Mobile and AT&T say US reg­u­la­tors should drop a plan to re­quire un­lock­ing of phones within 60 days of ac­ti­va­tion, claim­ing that lock­ing phones to a car­ri­er’s net­work makes it pos­si­ble to pro­vide cheaper hand­sets to con­sumers. If the Commission man­dates a uni­form un­lock­ing pol­icy, it is con­sumers—not providers—who stand to lose the most,” T-Mobile al­leged in an October 17 fil­ing with the Federal Communications Commission.

The pro­posed rule has sup­port from con­sumer ad­vo­cacy groups who say it will give users more choice and lower their costs. T-Mobile has been crit­i­cized for lock­ing phones for up to a year, which makes it im­pos­si­ble to use a phone on a ri­val’s net­work. T-Mobile claims that with a 60-day un­lock­ing rule, consumers risk los­ing ac­cess to the ben­e­fits of free or heav­ily sub­si­dized hand­sets be­cause the pro­posal would force providers to re­duce the line-up of their most com­pelling hand­set of­fers.”

If the pro­posed rule is en­acted, T-Mobile es­ti­mates that its pre­paid cus­tomers, for ex­am­ple, would see sub­si­dies re­duced by 40 per­cent to 70 per­cent for both its lower and higher-end de­vices, such as the Moto G, Samsung A15, and iPhone 12,” the car­rier said. A hand­set un­lock­ing man­date would also leave providers lit­tle choice but to limit their hand­set of­fers to lower cost and of­ten lesser per­form­ing hand­sets.”

T-Mobile and other car­ri­ers are re­spond­ing to a call for pub­lic com­ments that be­gan af­ter the FCC ap­proved a Notice of Proposed Rulemaking (NPRM) in a 5–0 vote. The FCC is propos­ing to re­quire all mo­bile wire­less ser­vice providers to un­lock hand­sets 60 days af­ter a con­sumer’s hand­set is ac­ti­vated with the provider, un­less within the 60-day pe­riod the ser­vice provider de­ter­mines the hand­set was pur­chased through fraud.”

When the FCC pro­posed the 60-day un­lock­ing rule in July 2024, the agency crit­i­cized T-Mobile for lock­ing pre­paid phones for a year. The NPRM pointed out that T-Mobile re­cently in­creased its lock­ing pe­riod for one of its brands, Metro by T-Mobile, from 180 days to 365 days.”

T-Mobile’s pol­icy says the car­rier will only un­lock mo­bile de­vices on pre­paid plans if at least 365 days… have passed since the de­vice was ac­ti­vated on the T-Mobile net­work.”

You bought your phone, you should be able to take it to any provider you want,” FCC Chairwoman Jessica Rosenworcel said when the FCC pro­posed the rule. Some providers al­ready op­er­ate this way. Others do not. In fact, some have re­cently in­creased the time their cus­tomers must wait un­til they can un­lock their de­vice by as much as 100 per­cent.”

T-Mobile ex­ec­u­tives, who also ar­gue that the FCC lacks au­thor­ity to im­pose the pro­posed rule, met with FCC of­fi­cials last week to ex­press their con­cerns.

T-Mobile is pas­sion­ate about win­ning cus­tomers for life, and ex­plained how its hand­set un­lock­ing poli­cies greatly ben­e­fit our cus­tomers,” the car­rier said in its post-meet­ing fil­ing. Our poli­cies al­low us to de­liver ac­cess to high-speed mo­bile broad­band on a na­tion­wide 5G net­work via hand­sets that are free or heav­ily dis­counted off the man­u­fac­tur­er’s sug­gested re­tail price. T-Mobile’s un­lock­ing poli­cies are trans­par­ent, and there is ab­solutely no ev­i­dence of con­sumer harm stem­ming from these poli­cies. T-Mobile’s cur­rent un­lock­ing poli­cies also help T-Mobile com­bat hand­set theft and fraud by so­phis­ti­cated, in­ter­na­tional crim­i­nal or­ga­ni­za­tions.”

For post­paid users, T-Mobile says it al­lows un­lock­ing of fully paid-off phones that have been ac­tive for at least 40 days. But given the 365-day lock on pre­paid users, T-Mobile’s over­all pol­icy is more oner­ous than those of other car­ri­ers. T-Mobile has also faced an­gry cus­tomers be­cause of a re­cent de­ci­sion to raise prices on plans that were ad­ver­tised as hav­ing a life­time price lock.

AT&T en­ables un­lock­ing of paid-off phones af­ter 60 days for post­paid users and af­ter six months for pre­paid users. AT&T lodged sim­i­lar com­plaints as T-Mobile, say­ing in an October 7 fil­ing that the FCCs pro­posed rules would mak[e] hand­sets less af­ford­able for con­sumers, es­pe­cially those in low-in­come house­holds,” and exacerbate hand­set ar­bi­trage, fraud, and traf­fick­ing.

AT&T told the FCC that requiring providers to un­lock hand­sets be­fore they are paid-off would ul­ti­mately harm con­sumers by cre­at­ing up­ward pres­sure on hand­set prices and dis­in­cen­tives to fi­nance hand­sets on flex­i­ble terms.” If the FCC im­ple­ments any rules, it should main­tain existing con­trac­tual arrange­ments be­tween cus­tomers and providers, en­sure that providers have at least 180 days to de­tect fraud be­fore un­lock­ing a de­vice, and in­clude at least a 24-month pe­riod for providers to im­ple­ment any new rules,” AT&T said.

Verizon, which al­ready faces un­lock­ing rules be­cause of re­quire­ments im­posed on spec­trum li­censes it owns, au­to­mat­i­cally un­locks phones af­ter 60 days for pre­paid and post­paid users. Among the three ma­jor car­ri­ers, Verizon is the most amenable to the FCCs new rules.

An October 18 fil­ing sup­port­ing a strict un­lock­ing rule was sub­mit­ted by nu­mer­ous con­sumer ad­vo­cacy groups in­clud­ing Public Knowledge, New America’s Open Technology Institute, Consumer Reports, the National Consumers League, the National Consumer Law Center, and the National Digital Inclusion Alliance.

Wireless users are sub­ject to un­nec­es­sary re­stric­tions in the form of locked de­vices, which tie them to their ser­vice providers even when bet­ter op­tions may be avail­able. Handset lock­ing prac­tices limit con­sumer free­dom and lessen com­pe­ti­tion by cre­at­ing an ar­ti­fi­cial tech­no­log­i­cal bar­rier to switch­ing providers,” the groups said.

The groups cited the Verizon rules as a model and urged the FCC to re­quire that de­vice un­lock­ing is truly au­to­matic—that is, un­locked af­ter the req­ui­site time pe­riod with­out any ad­di­tional ac­tions of the con­sumer.” Carriers should not be al­lowed to lock phones for longer than 60 days even when a phone is on a fi­nanc­ing plan with out­stand­ing pay­ments, the groups’ let­ter said:

Providers should be re­quired to tran­si­tion out of sell­ing de­vices with­out this [automatic un­lock­ing] ca­pa­bil­ity and the in­dus­try-wide rule should be the same as the one pro­tect­ing Verizon cus­tomers to­day: af­ter the ex­pi­ra­tion of the ini­tial pe­riod, the hand­set must au­to­mat­i­cally un­lock re­gard­less of whether: (1) the cus­tomer asks for the hand­set to be un­locked or (2) the hand­set is fully paid off. Removing this bar­rier to switch­ing will make the stan­dard sim­ple for con­sumers and en­cour­age providers to com­pete more vig­or­ously on mo­bile ser­vice price, qual­ity, and in­no­va­tion.

In an October 2 fil­ing, Verizon said it sup­ports a uni­form ap­proach to hand­set un­lock­ing that al­lows all wire­less providers to lock wire­less hand­sets for a rea­son­able pe­riod of time to limit fraud and to en­able de­vice sub­si­dies, fol­lowed by au­to­matic un­lock­ing ab­sent ev­i­dence of fraud.”

Verizon said 60 days should be the min­i­mum for post­paid de­vices so that car­ri­ers have time to de­tect fraud and theft, and that a longer, 180-day lock­ing pe­riod for pre­paid is nec­es­sary to en­able wire­less providers to con­tinue of­fer­ing sub­si­dies that make phones af­ford­able for pre­paid cus­tomers.” Regardless of what time frame the FCC chooses, Verizon said a uni­form un­lock­ing pol­icy that ap­plies to all providers… will ben­e­fit both con­sumers and com­pe­ti­tion.”

While the FCC is likely to im­pose an un­lock­ing rule, one ques­tion is whether it will ap­ply when a car­rier has pro­vided a dis­counted phone. The FCCs NPRM asked the pub­lic for comment on the im­pact of a 60-day un­lock­ing re­quire­ment in con­nec­tion with ser­vice providers’ in­cen­tives to of­fer dis­counted hand­sets for post­paid and pre­paid ser­vice plans.”

The FCC ac­knowl­edged Verizon’s ar­gu­ment that providers may rely on hand­set lock­ing to sus­tain their abil­ity to of­fer hand­set sub­si­dies and that such sub­si­dies may be par­tic­u­larly im­por­tant in pre­paid en­vi­ron­ments.” But the FCC noted that pub­lic in­ter­est groups argue that locked hand­sets tied to pre­paid plans can dis­ad­van­tage low-in­come cus­tomers most of all since they may not have the re­sources to switch ser­vice providers or pur­chase new hand­sets.”

The pub­lic in­ter­est groups also note that un­locked hand­sets facilitate a ro­bust sec­ondary mar­ket for used de­vices, pro­vid­ing con­sumers with more af­ford­able op­tions,” the NPRM said.

The FCC says it can im­pose phone-un­lock­ing rules us­ing its le­gal au­thor­ity un­der Title III of the Communications Act to pro­tect the pub­lic in­ter­est through spec­trum li­cens­ing and reg­u­la­tions to re­quire mo­bile wire­less ser­vice providers to pro­vide hand­set un­lock­ing.” The FCC said it pre­vi­ously re­lied on the same Title III au­thor­ity when it im­posed the un­lock­ing rules on 700 MHz C Block spec­trum li­censes pur­chased by Verizon.

T-Mobile told the FCC in a fil­ing last month that none of the litany of Title III pro­vi­sions cited in the NPRM sup­port the ex­pan­sive au­thor­ity as­serted here to reg­u­late con­sumer hand­sets (rather than telecom­mu­ni­ca­tions ser­vices).” T-Mobile also said that the Commission’s le­gal vul­ner­a­bil­i­ties on this score are only mag­ni­fied in light of re­cent Supreme Court prece­dent.”

The Supreme Court re­cently over­turned the 40-year-old Chevron prece­dent that gave agen­cies like the FCC ju­di­cial def­er­ence when in­ter­pret­ing am­bigu­ous laws. The end of Chevron makes it harder for agen­cies to is­sue reg­u­la­tions with­out ex­plicit au­tho­riza­tion from Congress. This is a po­ten­tial prob­lem for the FCC in its fight to re­vive net neu­tral­ity rules, which are cur­rently blocked by a court or­der pend­ing the out­come of lit­i­ga­tion.

Jon is a Senior IT Reporter for Ars Technica. He cov­ers the tele­com in­dus­try, Federal Communications Commission rule­mak­ings, broad­band con­sumer af­fairs, court cases, and gov­ern­ment reg­u­la­tion of the tech in­dus­try.

To the as­ton­ish­ment of fore­cast­ers, a tiny hur­ri­cane just sprang up near Cuba

Solar power from space? Actually, it might hap­pen in a cou­ple of years.

...

Read the original on arstechnica.com »

6 152 shares, 63 trendiness

Srinivasa Ramanujan Was a Genius. Math Is Still Catching Up.

One af­ter­noon in January 2011, Hussein Mourtada leapt onto his desk and started danc­ing. He was­n’t alone: Some of the grad­u­ate stu­dents who shared his Paris of­fice were there, too. But he did­n’t care. The math­e­mati­cian re­al­ized that he could fi­nally con­firm a sneak­ing sus­pi­cion he’d first had while writ­ing his doc­toral dis­ser­ta­tion, which he’d fin­ished a few months ear­lier. He’d been study­ing spe­cial points, called sin­gu­lar­i­ties, where curves cross them­selves or come to sharp turns. Now he had un­ex­pect­edly found what he’d been look­ing for, a way to prove that these sin­gu­lar­i­ties had a sur­pris­ingly deep un­der­ly­ing struc­ture. Hidden within that struc­ture were mys­te­ri­ous math­e­mat­i­cal state­ments first writ­ten down a cen­tury ear­lier by a young Indian math­e­mati­cian named Srinivasa Ramanujan. They had come to him in a dream.

Ramanujan brings life to the myth of the self-taught ge­nius. He grew up poor and un­e­d­u­cated and did much of his re­search while iso­lated in south­ern India, barely able to af­ford food. In 1912, when he was 24, he be­gan to send a se­ries of let­ters to promi­nent math­e­mati­cians. These were mostly ig­nored, but one re­cip­i­ent, the English math­e­mati­cian G. H. Hardy, cor­re­sponded with Ramanujan for a year and even­tu­ally per­suaded him to come to England, smooth­ing the way with the colo­nial bu­reau­cra­cies.

It be­came ap­par­ent to Hardy and his col­leagues that Ramanujan could sense math­e­mat­i­cal truths — could ac­cess en­tire worlds — that oth­ers sim­ply could not. (Hardy, a math­e­mat­i­cal gi­ant in his own right, is said to have quipped that his great­est con­tri­bu­tion to math­e­mat­ics was the dis­cov­ery of Ramanujan.) Before Ramanujan died in 1920 at the age of 32, he came up with thou­sands of el­e­gant and sur­pris­ing re­sults, of­ten with­out proof. He was fond of say­ing that his equa­tions had been be­stowed on him by the gods.

More than 100 years later, math­e­mati­cians are still try­ing to catch up to Ramanujan’s di­vine ge­nius, as his vi­sions ap­pear again and again in dis­parate cor­ners of the world of math­e­mat­ics.

Ramanujan is per­haps most fa­mous for com­ing up with par­ti­tion iden­ti­ties, equa­tions about the dif­fer­ent ways you can break a whole num­ber up into smaller parts (such as 7 = 5 + 1 + 1). In the 1980s, math­e­mati­cians be­gan to find deep and sur­pris­ing con­nec­tions be­tween these equa­tions and other ar­eas of math­e­mat­ics: in sta­tis­ti­cal me­chan­ics and the study of phase tran­si­tions, in knot the­ory and string the­ory, in num­ber the­ory and rep­re­sen­ta­tion the­ory and the study of sym­me­tries.

Most re­cently, they’ve ap­peared in Mourtada’s work on curves and sur­faces that are de­fined by al­ge­braic equa­tions, an area of study called al­ge­braic geom­e­try. Mourtada and his col­lab­o­ra­tors have spent more than a decade try­ing to bet­ter un­der­stand that link, and to ex­ploit it to un­cover rafts of brand-new iden­ti­ties that re­sem­ble those Ramanujan wrote down.

It turned out that these kinds of re­sults have ba­si­cally oc­curred in al­most every branch of math­e­mat­ics. That’s an amaz­ing thing,” said Ole Warnaar of the University of Queensland in Australia. It’s not just a happy co­in­ci­dence. I don’t want to sound re­li­gious, but the math­e­mat­i­cal god is try­ing to tell us some­thing.”

Ramanujan’s math­e­mat­i­cal prowess was ob­vi­ous to those who knew him. Without for­mal train­ing, he ex­celled; by the time he was in high school he had de­voured ad­vanced, though of­ten out­dated, text­books, and was do­ing in­de­pen­dent re­search on dif­fer­ent kinds of nu­mer­i­cal prop­er­ties and pat­terns.

In 1904, he was granted a full schol­ar­ship to the Government Arts College in Kumbakonam, the small city where he had grown up, in what is now the Indian state of Tamil Nadu. But he ig­nored all sub­jects be­sides math and lost his schol­ar­ship within a year. He later en­rolled in an­other uni­ver­sity, this time in Madras (now Chennai), the provin­cial cap­i­tal some 250 kilo­me­ters north. Again he flunked out.

He con­tin­ued his re­search on his own for years, of­ten while in poor health. During that time, he tu­tored stu­dents in math to sup­port him­self. Eventually he se­cured a job as a clerk at the Madras Port Trust in 1912. He pur­sued math­e­mat­ics on the side and pub­lished some of his re­sults in Indian jour­nals.

Hoping to get some of his work into more pres­ti­gious and widely read pub­li­ca­tions, Ramanujan wrote let­ters to sev­eral British math­e­mati­cians, en­clos­ing pages of find­ings for their re­view. I have not trod­den through the con­ven­tional reg­u­lar course which is fol­lowed in a uni­ver­sity course,” he wrote, but I am strik­ing out a new path for my­self.” Among the re­cip­i­ents was Hardy, an ex­pert in num­ber the­ory and analy­sis at the University of Cambridge.

Hardy was shocked at what he saw. Ramanujan had iden­ti­fied and then solved a num­ber of con­tin­ued frac­tions — ex­pres­sions that can be writ­ten as in­fi­nite nests of frac­tions within frac­tions, such as:

They defeated me com­pletely; I had never seen any­thing in the least like them be­fore,” Hardy later wrote. They must be true be­cause, if they were not true, no one would have had the imag­i­na­tion to in­vent them.” The for­mu­las, un­proved, were so strik­ing that they in­spired Hardy to of­fer Ramanujan a fel­low­ship at Cambridge. In 1914, Ramanujan ar­rived in England, and for the next five years he stud­ied and col­lab­o­rated with Hardy.

One of Ramanujan’s first tasks was to prove a gen­eral state­ment about his con­tin­ued frac­tions. To do so, he needed to prove two other state­ments. But he could­n’t. Neither could Hardy, nor could any of the col­leagues he reached out to.

It turned out that they did­n’t need to. The state­ments had been proved 20 years ear­lier by a lit­tle-known English math­e­mati­cian named L. J. Rogers. Rogers wrote poorly, and at the time the proofs were pub­lished no one paid any at­ten­tion. (Rogers was con­tent to do his re­search in rel­a­tive ob­scu­rity, play pi­ano, gar­den and ap­ply his spare time to a va­ri­ety of other pur­suits.) Ramanujan un­cov­ered this work in 1917, and the pair of state­ments later be­came known as the Rogers-Ramanujan iden­ti­ties.

Amid Ramanujan’s prodi­gious out­put, these state­ments stand out. They have car­ried through the decades and across nearly all of math­e­mat­ics. They are the seeds that math­e­mati­cians con­tinue to sow, grow­ing bril­liant new gar­dens seem­ingly wher­ever they fall.

Ramanujan fell ill and re­turned to India in 1919, where he died the next year. It would fall to oth­ers to ex­plore the world his iden­ti­ties had re­vealed.

Hussein Mourtada grew up in the 1980s in Lebanon, in a small city called Baalbek. As a teenager, he did­n’t like study­ing and pre­ferred to play: soc­cer, bil­liards, bas­ket­ball. Math, too. It looked like a game,” he said. And I liked play­ing.”

As an un­der­grad­u­ate at the Lebanese University in Beirut, he stud­ied both law and math­e­mat­ics, with an eye to a le­gal ca­reer. But he soon found that while he en­joyed the philo­soph­i­cal as­pects of law, he did not en­joy it in prac­tice. He turned his at­ten­tion to math, where he was par­tic­u­larly drawn to the com­mu­nity. As a child, his teach­ers and class­mates were what ex­cited him about go­ing to school, even though he of­ten fell asleep dur­ing class. As a bud­ding math­e­mati­cian, I had the im­pres­sion that these are beau­ti­ful peo­ple,” he said. They are hon­est. You need to be hon­est with your­self to be a math­e­mati­cian. Otherwise, it does­n’t work.”

He moved to France for his doc­tor­ate and started to fo­cus on al­ge­braic geom­e­try — the study of al­ge­braic va­ri­eties, or shapes cut out by poly­no­mial equa­tions. These are equa­tions that can be writ­ten as sums of vari­ables raised to whole-num­ber pow­ers. A line, for in­stance, is cut out by the equa­tion x + y = 0, a cir­cle by x2 + y2 = 1, a fig­ure eight by x4 = x2 − y2. While the line and cir­cle are com­pletely smooth, the fig­ure eight has a point where it in­ter­sects it­self — a sin­gu­lar­ity.

It’s easy to spot sin­gu­lar­i­ties when you’re deal­ing with shapes that you can draw on a sheet of pa­per. But higher-di­men­sional al­ge­braic va­ri­eties are far more com­pli­cated and im­pos­si­ble to vi­su­al­ize. Algebraic geome­ters are in the busi­ness of un­der­stand­ing their sin­gu­lar­i­ties, too.

They’ve de­vel­oped all sorts of tools to do this. One dates back to the math­e­mati­cian John Nash, who in the 1960s started study­ing re­lated ob­jects called arc spaces. Nash would take a point, or sin­gu­lar­ity, and de­fine in­fi­nitely many short tra­jec­to­ries — lit­tle arcs — that passed through it. By look­ing at all these short tra­jec­to­ries to­gether, he could test how smooth his va­ri­ety was at that point. If you want to see if it’s smooth, you want to pet it,” said Gleb Pogudin of the École Polytechnique in France.

In prac­ti­cal terms, an arc space pro­vides an in­fi­nite col­lec­tion of poly­no­mial equa­tions. This is re­ally the thing that Mourtada is ex­pert in: un­der­stand­ing the mean­ing of those equa­tions,” said Bernard Teissier, a col­league of Mourtada’s at the Institute of Mathematics of Jussieu in Paris. Because these equa­tions can be very com­pli­cated. But they have a cer­tain mu­sic to them. There is a lot of struc­ture which gov­erns the na­ture of these equa­tions, and he’s just the per­son, I think, who best lis­tens to this mu­sic and un­der­stands what it means.”

...

Read the original on www.quantamagazine.org »

7 152 shares, 8 trendiness

Intelsat 33e breaks up in geostationary orbit

TAMPA, Fla. — The Intelsat 33e satel­lite has bro­ken up in geo­sta­tion­ary or­bit (GEO) and lost power, ceas­ing com­mu­ni­ca­tions ser­vices for cus­tomers across Europe, Africa and parts of Asia Pacific.

Intelsat said in an Oct. 19 news re­lease it is work­ing with satel­lite maker Boeing to ad­dress an anom­aly that emerged ear­lier that day, but believe it is un­likely that the satel­lite will be re­cov­er­able.” An Intelsat spokesper­son said the satel­lite was not in­sured at the time of the is­sue.

The U. S. Space Force re­ported Oct. 19 it is track­ing around 20 pieces of de­bris as­so­ci­ated with the space­craft.

U. S. Space Forces-Space (S4S) has con­firmed the breakup of Intelsat 33E (#41748, 2016-053B) in GEO on October 19, 2024, at ap­prox­i­mately 0430 UTC,” states an alert posted on SpaceTrack, the U.S. Department of Defense’s space-track­ing plat­form.

Currently track­ing around 20 as­so­ci­ated pieces — analy­sis on­go­ing. S4S has ob­served no im­me­di­ate threats and is con­tin­u­ing to con­duct rou­tine con­junc­tion as­sess­ments to sup­port the safety and sus­tain­abil­ity of the space do­main.”

Douglas Hendrix, CEO of ExoAnalytic Solutions, said the U. S.-based space-track­ing com­pany iden­ti­fied 57 pieces of de­bris Oct. 21 as­so­ci­ated with the breakup.

We are warn­ing op­er­a­tors of any space­craft that we think are at risk of col­li­sion,” Hendrix said via email.

A snap­shot of Intelsat 33e’s break-up taken Oct. 19 by U. K.-based Spaceflux. 44071 and 58698 are the WGS 10 (USA 291) and Ovzon-3 satel­lites, re­spec­tively, which Spaceflux said are un­likely in dan­ger of be­ing hit by the de­bris. The prob­lem is that there is a lot of un­cer­tainty re­gard­ing the or­bits of these frag­ments at the mo­ment,” Spaceflux spokesper­son Viktoria Urban said Oct. 21. They can be po­ten­tially dan­ger­ous for other satel­lites but we do not know that yet.” Credit: Spaceflux.

Intelsat said it is work­ing to move cus­tomers to other satel­lites in Intelsat’s fleet or space­craft op­er­ated by third par­ties.

Intelsat 33e launched in August 2016 and en­tered ser­vice in January 2017 at 60 de­grees East, about three months later than planned fol­low­ing an is­sue with its pri­mary thruster.

A sec­ond propul­sion is­sue that emerged dur­ing in-or­bit tests helped knock off around 3.5 years from the satel­lite’s ini­tially es­ti­mated 15-year lifes­pan.

Intelsat 33e is the sec­ond in Intelsat’s EpicNG (next-generation) se­ries of high-through­put satel­lites.

The first, Intelsat-29e, was de­clared a to­tal loss in 2019 af­ter just three years in or­bit. That fail­ure was pinned on ei­ther a me­te­oroid im­pact or a wiring flaw that led to an elec­tro­sta­tic dis­charge fol­low­ing height­ened so­lar weather ac­tiv­ity.

This ar­ti­cle was up­dated Oct. 21 with more de­tails about the in­ci­dent.

...

Read the original on spacenews.com »

8 147 shares, 10 trendiness

IOCCC Flight Simulator

Be sure to visit our Facebook Page.

The IOCCC Flight Simulator was the win­ning en­try in the 1998 International Obfuscated C Code Contest. It is a flight sim­u­la­tor in un­der 2 kilo­bytes of code, com­plete with rel­a­tively ac­cu­rate 6-degree-of-freedom dy­nam­ics, load­able wire­frame scenery, and a small in­stru­ment panel.

IOCCC Flight Simulator runs on Unix-like sys­tems with X Windows. As per con­test rules, it is in the pub­lic do­main.

You have just stepped out of the real world and into the vir­tual. You are now sit­ting in the cock­pit of a Piper Cherokee air­plane, head­ing north, fly­ing 1000 feet above ground level.

Use the key­board to fly the air­plane. The ar­row keys rep­re­sent the con­trol stick. Press the Up Arrow key to push the stick for­ward. Press the left ar­row key to move the stick left, and so on. Press Enter to re-cen­ter the stick. Use Page Up and Page Down in­crease and de­crease the throt­tle, re­spec­tively. (The rud­der is au­to­mat­i­cally co­or­di­nated with the turn rate, so rud­der ped­als are not rep­re­sented.)

On your dis­play, you will see on the bot­tom left cor­ner three in­stru­ments. The first is the air­speed in­di­ca­tor; it tells you how fast you’re go­ing in knots. The sec­ond is the head­ing in­di­ca­tor, or com­pass. 0 is north, 90 is east, 180 is south, 270 is west. The third in­stru­ment is the al­time­ter, which mea­sures your height above ground level in feet.

cat hori­zon.sc pitts­burgh.sc | ./banks

banks is the name of the pro­gram (a quirk of IOCCC rules, and no pun in­tended). hori­zon.sc and pitts­burgh.sc are scenery files.

* The air­plane is mod­eled as a six de­gree-of-free­dom rigid body, ac­cu­rately re­flect­ing its dy­nam­ics (for nor­mal flight con­di­tions, at least).

* Fly through a vir­tual 3-D world, while sit­ting at your X con­sole.

* Head-up dis­play con­tains three in­stru­ments: a true air­speed in­di­ca­tor, a head­ing in­di­ca­tor (compass), and an al­time­ter.

* Flight con­trols may be mapped to any keys at com­pile time by re­defin­ing the macros in the build file. Nice if your key­board does­n’t have ar­row keys.

* Time step size can be set at com­pile time. This is use­ful to re­duce flicker on net­work X con­nec­tions. (But be care­ful: step sizes longer than about 0.03 sec­onds tend to have nu­mer­i­cal sta­bil­ity prob­lems.)

* Airplane never runs out of fuel!

Each of the files is a scenery file. The sim­u­la­tor pro­gram reads in the scenery from stan­dard in­put on startup. You may in­put more than one scenery file, as long as there are less than 1000 to­tal lines of in­put.

Here is a brief de­scrip­tion of the scenery files:

* hori­zon.sc — A hori­zon, noth­ing more. You will prob­a­bly al­ways want to in­put this piece of scenery.

* moun­tains.sc — An al­ter­nate hori­zon; a lit­tle more moun­tain­ous.

* pitts­burgh.sc — Scenery of down­town Pittsburgh. The down­town area is ini­tially lo­cated to your right.

* bb.sc — Simple ob­sta­cle course. Try to fly over the build­ings and un­der the bridges.

* pyra­mids.sc — Fly over the tombs of the an­cient Pharaohs in this (fictitious) Egyptian land­scape.

A few ex­am­ples of how to in­put scenery:

cat hori­zon.sc pitts­burgh.sc | ./banks

cat moun­tains.sc bb.sc | ./banks

cat moun­tains.sc river.sc pyra­mids.sc | ./banks

You can sim­u­late fly­ing through a cloud bank as well:

./banks < /dev/null

You will usu­ally want at least a hori­zon, though.

The for­mat of scenery files is sim­ple, by the way. They’re just a list of 3-D co­or­di­nates, and the sim­u­la­tor sim­ply draws line seg­ments from point to point as listed in the scenery file. 0 0 0 is used to end a se­ries of con­sec­u­tive line seg­ments. Note that in the co­or­di­nate sys­tem used, the third co­or­di­nate rep­re­sents al­ti­tude in a neg­a­tive sense: neg­a­tive num­bers are pos­i­tive al­ti­tudes.

I’m sure you’ll be mak­ing your own scenery files very soon!!!

Several op­tions must be passed to the com­piler to make the build work. The pro­vided build file has the ap­pro­pri­ate op­tions set to de­fault val­ues. Use this sec­tion if you want to com­pile with dif­fer­ent op­tions.

To map a key to a con­trol, you must pass an op­tion to the com­piler in the for­mat -Dcontrol=key”. The pos­si­ble con­trols you can map are de­scribed in the table be­low:

Control Description Default Key

IT Open throt­tle XK_Page_Up

DT Close throt­tle XK_Page_Down

FD Move stick for­ward XK_Up

BK Move stick back XK_Down

LT Move stick left XK_Left

RT Move stick right XK_Right

CS Center stick XK_Enter

Values for the pos­si­ble keys can be found in the X Windows header file . This file is most likely a cross-ref­er­ence to an­other header,

You must map all seven con­trols to keys at com­pile time, or the com­pi­la­tion will fail.

For ex­am­ple, to map Center Stick to the space-bar, the com­pile op­tion would be -DCS=XK_space”.

To set the time step size, you must pass the fol­low­ing op­tion to the com­piler: -Ddt=duration”, where dt is lit­eral, and where du­ra­tion is the time in sec­onds you want the time step to be.

Two things to keep in mind when se­lect­ing a time step. Time steps that are too large (more than about 0.03) will cause nu­mer­i­cal sta­bil­ity prob­lems and should be avoided. Setting the time step to be smaller than your clock res­o­lu­tion will slow down the sim­u­la­tor, be­cause the sys­tem pauses for more time than the sim­u­la­tor ex­pects.

The best ad­vice is to set time step size to your sys­tem timer res­o­lu­tion. Try a longer pe­riod if you’re get­ting too much flicker.

Here we are fly­ing to­wards Downtown Pittsburgh. We can see the Point, sev­eral build­ings in­clud­ing the USX tower, and sev­eral bridges in­clud­ing the Smithfield Street bridge. We see three in­stru­ments near the bot­tom.

IOCCC stands for International Obfuscated C Code Contest.” It is an quasi-an­nual con­test to see who can write the most un­read­able, un­in­tel­li­gi­ble, un­managable, but le­gal C pro­gram.

In the 1998 IOCCC, My flight sim­u­la­tor won the Best of Show” prize. Here is the source code to the pro­gram:

#include

Note that this pro­gram will not com­pile out-of-the-box. It re­quires cer­tain com­pile-time pa­ra­me­ters. The fol­lo­ing script builds it on my Linux sys­tem:

#! /bin/sh

cc banks.c -o banks -DIT=XK_Page_Up -DDT=XK_Page_Down \

-DUP=XK_Up -DDN=XK_Down -DLT=XK_Left -DRT=XK_Right \

-DCS=XK_Return -Ddt=0.02 -lm -lX11 -L/usr/X11R6/lib

If you want to try this pro­gram, I sug­gest you down­load the 1998 IOCCC Winners Distribution.

One of the rules of the con­test was that the pro­gram could not be longer than 1536 bytes (excluding spaces, tabs, new­lines, semi­colons, and braces). Needless to say, cram­ming a flight sim­u­la­tor into such a small file was fairly dif­fi­cult. I will say that if it weren’t for the won­der­ful prop­erty of or­thog­o­nal ma­tri­ces, this flight sim­u­la­tor would not have been pos­si­ble.

* The IOCCC Simulator ap­peared in a book, Calculated Bets by Steve Skiena.

* Wikipedia has a list­ing of IOCCC Simluator in its IOCCC Entry.

* The of­fi­cial International Obfuscated C Code Contest web­site

* IOCCC Flight Simulator’s Facebook page. (I made this page be­cause ran­dom peo­ple around the world would send me friend re­quests and it was creep­ing me out.)

I do not dis­trib­ute this pro­gram my­self. If you want it, you can down­load the 1998 IOCCC Winners Distribution. The dis­tri­b­u­tion comes with a dozen or so other win­ning en­tries, all quite in­ter­est­ing pro­grams.

Note that this is a source dis­tri­b­u­tion, and you will have com­pile it to run it. I’ve tested it on some ver­sions of Linux, AIX, Irix, and Sun.

IOCCC Flight Simulator source code is in the pub­lic do­main; there are no copy­right re­stric­tions on it what­so­ever. However, the win­ners dis­tri­b­u­tion has been copy­righted by the IOCCC judges. See the hint files in the dis­tri­b­u­tion for de­tails.

...

Read the original on blog.aerojockey.com »

9 139 shares, 5 trendiness

Software Engineer Titles Have (Almost) Lost All Their Meaning

Remember when be­ing a Senior Software Engineer” ac­tu­ally meant some­thing? I do, and I can’t help but feel nos­tal­gic for that clar­ity. In re­cent years, our in­dus­try has wit­nessed ram­pant ti­tle in­fla­tion, turn­ing what used to be a clear-cut ju­nior-mid-se­nior pro­gres­sion into a con­fus­ing pa­rade of in­flated roles.

The senior” ti­tle, once a badge of sub­stan­tial ex­pe­ri­ence and ex­per­tise, has been par­tic­u­larly de­val­ued. Today, de­vel­op­ers are be­ing crowned senior” faster than ever, of­ten with just three to four years un­der their belts. It’s as if the path to se­nior­ity, once a marathon of skill-build­ing and di­verse ex­pe­ri­ences, has turned into a sprint­er’s dash.

This ex­plo­sion of grandiose ti­tles is­n’t just con­fus­ing—it’s erod­ing the mean­ing of ca­reer mile­stones in tech. Each new ti­tle tries to outdo the last in im­pres­sive­ness, while para­dox­i­cally mean­ing less and less. For every­one in­volved—from job seek­ers to hir­ing man­agers—this in­fla­tion has mud­died the wa­ters of pro­fes­sional pro­gres­sion and recog­ni­tion.

Being a se­nior en­gi­neer meant far more than just log­ging years on the job. It was a ti­tle earned through a di­verse set of ex­pe­ri­ences and chal­lenges that shaped not just their tech­ni­cal skills, but their en­tire ap­proach to soft­ware de­vel­op­ment.

A true se­nior en­gi­neer is a bat­tle-tested prob­lem solver. They’ve faced and con­quered com­plex tech­ni­cal chal­lenges across mul­ti­ple pro­jects, deal­ing with more than just tricky bugs. These are the ar­chi­tects who’ve un­tan­gled sys­tem-wide is­sues that re­quire deep un­der­stand­ing and cre­ative so­lu­tions. They’re the ones who can nav­i­gate and refac­tor sprawl­ing legacy code­bases with con­fi­dence, un­der­stand­ing the del­i­cate bal­ance be­tween main­tain­ing ex­ist­ing sys­tems and build­ing new ones.

Senior en­gi­neers have been through the cru­cible of ma­jor pro­duc­tion out­ages. They’ve felt the heat of a sys­tem melt­ing down in real-time and learned to stay calm un­der pres­sure. These ex­pe­ri­ences have taught them to di­ag­nose is­sues rapidly and lead a team through a cri­sis, mak­ing crit­i­cal de­ci­sions when every sec­ond counts.

But tech­ni­cal skills alone don’t make a se­nior en­gi­neer. They’re also ar­chi­tec­tural vi­sion­ar­ies who can see be­yond im­me­di­ate tasks to de­sign scal­able, main­tain­able sys­tems. Their de­ci­sions pos­i­tively im­pact pro­jects years down the line, show­cas­ing a level of fore­sight that only comes with ex­ten­sive ex­pe­ri­ence. They’ve de­vel­oped the soft skills to be ef­fec­tive men­tors and lead­ers, guid­ing ju­nior de­vel­op­ers not just in cod­ing, but in nav­i­gat­ing the com­plex land­scape of soft­ware de­vel­op­ment.

Perhaps most im­por­tantly, se­nior en­gi­neers re­main hum­ble and cu­ri­ous de­spite their ex­pe­ri­ence. They’re con­tin­u­ous learn­ers, adapt­ing to new tech­nolo­gies and method­olo­gies, al­ways ex­pand­ing their toolkit. They’ve de­vel­oped a strong sense of pro­fes­sional ethics, un­der­stand­ing the broader im­pli­ca­tions of their work and ad­vo­cat­ing for re­spon­si­ble de­vel­op­ment prac­tices.

This depth of ex­pe­ri­ence is­n’t typ­i­cally gained in just a few years. It’s forged through di­verse pro­jects, mul­ti­ple tech stacks, and yes, a fair share of fail­ures and lessons learned along the way.

The fierce com­pe­ti­tion for tal­ent has led com­pa­nies, es­pe­cially star­tups, to use ti­tles as a re­ten­tion tac­tic. Unable to al­ways match the salaries of­fered by tech gi­ants, these com­pa­nies re­sort to in­flat­ing ti­tles as a form of non-mon­e­tary com­pen­sa­tion. While this might seem like a clever short-term so­lu­tion, it’s cre­at­ing long-term prob­lems for the in­dus­try by di­lut­ing the mean­ing of these ti­tles.

The rise of pro­fes­sional net­work­ing plat­forms like LinkedIn has ex­ac­er­bated this is­sue. These plat­forms have turned ti­tles into per­sonal brand­ing tools, cre­at­ing im­mense pres­sure for in­di­vid­u­als to sport im­pres­sive-sound­ing roles. This LinkedIn Effect” has every­one, from fresh grad­u­ates to sea­soned pro­fes­sion­als, yearn­ing for ti­tles that look good on their pro­files, of­ten pri­or­i­tiz­ing ap­pear­ance over sub­stance.

HR de­part­ments, grap­pling with the in­creas­ing com­plex­ity of tech roles, have con­tributed to this prob­lem as well. In an at­tempt to ac­cu­rately cat­e­go­rize the myr­iad of spe­cial­ized po­si­tions in our rapidly evolv­ing field, they’ve cre­ated a pro­lif­er­a­tion of niche ti­tles. While these ti­tles might be de­scrip­tive, they’ve made it in­creas­ingly dif­fi­cult to com­pare roles across com­pa­nies, fur­ther mud­dy­ing the wa­ters of ca­reer pro­gres­sion.

Lastly, many com­pa­nies have be­gun us­ing ti­tle pro­mo­tions as a re­ten­tion strat­egy. The in­tent is to rec­og­nize and re­tain valu­able em­ploy­ees, but this ap­proach of­ten back­fires. When ti­tles are handed out like par­tic­i­pa­tion tro­phies, they cease to align with ac­tual growth in re­spon­si­bil­i­ties or skills. This mis­align­ment not only de­val­ues the ti­tles them­selves but also sets un­re­al­is­tic ex­pec­ta­tions for the newly pro­moted em­ploy­ees.

In essence, what we’re see­ing is a per­fect storm of mar­ket pres­sures, per­sonal brand­ing needs, or­ga­ni­za­tional chal­lenges, and short-sighted re­ten­tion strate­gies. Together, these fac­tors have in­flated ti­tles to the point where they risk los­ing their mean­ing en­tirely.

Title in­fla­tion is­n’t just about words on a busi­ness card or a LinkedIn pro­file. It’s a prob­lem that strikes at the heart of our in­dus­try’s in­tegrity and func­tion­al­ity. When we in­flate ti­tles, we’re es­sen­tially ly­ing to our­selves and each other about our ca­pa­bil­i­ties and ex­pe­ri­ence.

This de­cep­tion has real con­se­quences. It cre­ates a mis­match be­tween ex­pec­ta­tions and re­al­ity, lead­ing to sit­u­a­tions where peo­ple are placed in roles they’re not pre­pared for. Imagine a senior” en­gi­neer with three years of ex­pe­ri­ence try­ing to ar­chi­tect a com­plex sys­tem or men­tor ju­nior de­vel­op­ers. The po­ten­tial for fail­ure is high, and the stress on that in­di­vid­ual is im­mense.

For those in lead­er­ship po­si­tions, it’s para­mount to re­sist the temp­ta­tion of us­ing in­flated ti­tles as a quick fix for re­ten­tion or re­cruit­ment chal­lenges. Instead, fo­cus on cre­at­ing mean­ing­ful ca­reer pro­gres­sion frame­works that tie ad­vance­ments to con­crete skills and re­spon­si­bil­i­ties. Consider im­ple­ment­ing a sys­tem sim­i­lar to those used by larger tech com­pa­nies, where lev­els (like L3, L4, L5) pro­vide a more nu­anced view of se­nior­ity with­out re­sort­ing to ti­tle in­fla­tion.

Companies can take a stand by stan­dard­iz­ing their ti­tle struc­tures and be­ing trans­par­ent about what each level means. This could in­volve cre­at­ing de­tailed job de­scrip­tions that clearly out­line the ex­pec­ta­tions and re­spon­si­bil­i­ties for each role. By do­ing so, you not only pro­vide clar­ity for your em­ploy­ees but also con­tribute to a more stan­dard­ized in­dus­try-wide un­der­stand­ing of ti­tles.

HR de­part­ments have a crit­i­cal role to play. They can work on de­vel­op­ing more so­phis­ti­cated ways to cat­e­go­rize and com­pare roles across the in­dus­try. This might in­volve col­lab­o­rat­ing with tech leads to cre­ate stan­dard­ized skill ma­tri­ces that can be used to eval­u­ate can­di­dates and em­ploy­ees more ob­jec­tively.

Companies that re­sist ti­tle in­fla­tion gain a sig­nif­i­cant com­pet­i­tive edge. By main­tain­ing mean­ing­ful ti­tles, they at­tract and re­tain top tal­ent who value au­then­tic growth over in­flated roles. This leads to more ac­cu­rate hir­ing, im­proved team dy­nam­ics, and en­hanced pro­duc­tiv­ity. Realistic ti­tles also fos­ter trust, both in­ter­nally and with clients, po­si­tion­ing the com­pany as a bea­con of in­tegrity in the in­dus­try. Ultimately, com­pa­nies with well-de­fined, hon­est ti­tle struc­tures build stronger, more ca­pa­ble teams and a rep­u­ta­tion for ex­cel­lence that sets them apart in the mar­ket.

This ar­ti­cle was orig­i­nally pub­lished on

https://​www.trevor­lasn.com/​blog/​soft­ware-en­gi­neer-ti­tles-have-al­most-lost-all-their-mean­ing. It was writ­ten by a hu­man and pol­ished us­ing gram­mar tools for clar­ity.

...

Read the original on www.trevorlasn.com »

10 138 shares, 9 trendiness

How Scientists Started to Decode Birdsong

Skip to main con­tentFind any­thing you save across the site in your ac­count Language is said to make us hu­man. What if birds talk, too?“So­cial birds . . . are con­stantly chat­ting to each other,” Mike Webster, an an­i­mal-com­mu­ni­ca­tion ex­pert at Cornell, says. What in the hell are they say­ing?”On a driz­zly day in Grünau im Almtal, Austria, a gag­gle of grey­lag geese shared a peace­ful mo­ment on a grassy field near a stream. One goose, named Edes, was preen­ing qui­etly; oth­ers were rest­ing with their beaks pointed tail­ward, nes­tled into their feath­ers. Then a cam­ou­flaged speaker that sci­en­tists had placed nearby started to play. First came a recorded honk from an un­part­nered male goose named Joshua. Edes went on with his preen­ing. Next came a honk that was lower in pitch than the first, with a slight bray. Edes looked up. As the other geese re­mained tucked in their warm po­si­tions, in­cu­ri­ous, Edes scanned the field. He had just heard a recorded distance call” from his life part­ner, a fe­male goose whom sci­en­tists had named Bon Jovi. Edes and his fel­low-geese live near the Konrad Lorenz Research Center for Behavior and Cognition, which is named for a Nobel lau­re­ate whose im­print­ing ex­per­i­ments, in the nine­teen-thir­ties, con­vinced goslings that he was their mother. (They took to fol­low­ing him in a downy line.) Greylag geese in the area have been stud­ied con­tin­u­ally ever since. The di­rec­tor of the cen­ter, a bi­ol­o­gist and bird ecol­o­gist named Sonia Kleindorfer, showed me footage of Edes to demon­strate the sub­tlety of goose com­mu­ni­ca­tion.Geese main­tain elab­o­rate so­cial struc­tures, travel in fam­ily groups, and can nav­i­gate from Sweden to Spain. In a fight, an un­part­nered grey­lag goose has a higher heart rate than a part­nered one, and the heart rate of a re­cently wid­owed goose can re­main de­pressed for about a year. These birds have things to dis­cuss. Still, geese are not the Ciceros of the bird world. A lyre­bird sings long, elab­o­rate songs; ravens re­ally can say nevermore.” Geese are known for nasal honks. How much nu­ance can there be in a honk?Grey­lag geese, it turns out, have at least ten dif­fer­ent kinds of calls. We are com­pletely un­der­ap­pre­ci­at­ing the way they com­mu­ni­cate,” Kleindorfer told me. They give a de­par­ture call when they leave, and a con­tact call af­ter they ar­rive. They know if their al­lies are there, if the bold geese are there. There is so much in­for­ma­tion that geese are get­ting from calls.”Bird vo­cal­iza­tions are usu­ally di­vided into songs and calls, but these are wob­bly cat­e­gories. What is des­ig­nated a song in one species may be shorter in du­ra­tion than what, in an­other species, is termed a call. Onomatopoeic group­ings such as tseets, chirrups, rreey­oos, seeew-soooos, and dahs are also in­de­ter­mi­nate: peo­ple tran­scribe the same sounds in dif­fer­ent ways, and no bird ver­sion of the Académie Française ex­ists to ad­ju­di­cate. The vo­cal­iza­tions of birds are fun­da­men­tally in­com­men­su­rate with hu­man ones. We have a lar­ynx and two vo­cal cords; they have what’s called a sy­rinx, which is a bit like hav­ing two lar­ynxes that you can use at the same time.Klein­dor­fer, the daugh­ter of a math­e­mati­cian and an ac­tress, looks like a cross be­tween a hiker and the film star Sophia Loren. From February to April, she re­searches Darwin’s finches in the Galápagos; from September to December, song­birds in Australia; and, for the rest of the year, the geese out­side her of­fice door. Early in her ed­u­ca­tion, as an un­der­grad­u­ate at the University of Pennsylvania, she was taught that male song­birds sing, fe­males don’t, and if fe­males do sing it’s an er­ror.” The at­ti­tude at the time, she told me, was that females are drab, in­con­spic­u­ous, and quiet.” A few years af­ter earn­ing a Ph.D. in zo­ol­ogy at the University of Vienna, Kleindorfer took a job as a re­search bi­ol­o­gist at Flinders University, in Australia, where song­bird species orig­i­nally evolved. Imagine my sur­prise,” she told me. I heard all these fe­males singing songs as com­plex as the male songs.” Much of her en­su­ing ca­reer has fo­cussed on bird vo­cal­iza­tions that were ei­ther un­der­ap­pre­ci­ated or un­known.Klein­dor­fer de­cided to study bird eggs and early de­vel­op­ment, which were then ne­glected re­search top­ics. Maybe this was be­cause only fe­males have eggs and I was a woman in sci­ence,” she told me. I don’t have a bet­ter rea­son.” Kleindorfer had no­ticed that mus­tached-war­bler chicks seemed to re­spond to the alarm calls of adult war­blers, even though the think­ing at the time was that such calls were di­rected at other adults, or pos­si­bly at preda­tors. If I put a snake nearby, the parental alarm call made the chicks in the nest jump,” she said. If I put a marsh har­rier”—a hawk­like preda­tory bird—“nearby, the re­sponse to the parental alarm call was that the chicks would duck.” The chicks were re­spond­ing ap­pro­pri­ately to dif­fer­ent alarm calls—a sat­is­fy­ing find­ing.Klein­dor­fer also stud­ied the su­perb fairy wren, a song­bird that weighs about as much as a wal­nut and sports a flirty, up­right tail. Despite their fan­ci­ful names, fairy wrens are com­mon­place in Australia. They are so­cially monog­a­mous but sex­u­ally promis­cu­ous—they are es­sen­tially in open mar­riages—and they bring up their young col­lec­tively. Arguably, they have even more to chat about than geese do. Fairy-wren nests are about the size of cupped hu­man hands, built to con­tain pale, speck­led eggs that are smaller than thumb­nails. Kleindorfer and her team wired up nests with cam­eras and mi­cro­phones and soon dis­cov­ered some­thing that they had­n’t known to look for. The moth­ers in nests were pro­duc­ing an in­cu­ba­tion call—a call to the eggs,” she told me. It was like a lul­laby. Why would a mother bird make any sound that could at­tract preda­tors to the nest? Songbird em­bryos don’t have well-de­vel­oped ears, so this was com­pletely un­ex­pected,” she said. That started a twenty-year pro­ject—why is she call­ing to the eggs?”The team com­pared in­cu­ba­tion calls to the beg­ging calls of young chicks. It was very odd,” Kleindorfer re­called. Each nest had its own dis­tinct beg­ging call.” What’s more, each beg­ging call matched an el­e­ment from the moth­er’s in­cu­ba­tion call. This sug­gested, star­tlingly, that birds could learn a lit­eral mother tongue while still in ovo. (Humans do this, too; French and German ba­bies have dis­tinct cries.) Even foster” chicks, who as eggs were phys­i­cally moved from one nest to an­other, learned beg­ging calls from their fos­ter moth­ers, rather than from their ge­netic moth­ers. This was big news in the or­nithol­ogy world. The par­a­digm of how song­birds learn—af­ter hatch­ing, from their fa­ther’s song—was over­thrown,” she said. The same process was soon doc­u­mented in more song­bird species.Lan­guage is of­ten cited as the qual­ity that dis­tin­guishes us as hu­mans. When I asked Robert Berwick, an M.I.T. com­pu­ta­tional lin­guist, about birds, he ar­gued that they’re not try­ing to say any­thing in the sense of James Joyce try­ing to say some­thing.” Still, he and Kleindorfer both pointed out that hu­mans and song­birds share a trait that many an­i­mals lack: we are vocal learn­ers,” mean­ing that we can learn to make new sounds through­out our lives. (Bats, whales, dol­phins, and ele­phants can, too.) To me, the most amaz­ing thing is that every gen­er­a­tion of vo­cal learn­ers has its own sound,” Kleindorfer said. So, just like our English is dif­fer­ent from Shakespeare’s English, the song­birds, too, have very dif­fer­ent songs from five hun­dred years ago. I am sure of it.” We hu­mans have long tried, of­ten mis­tak­enly, to dif­fer­en­ti­ate our­selves from non­hu­man an­i­mals—by ar­gu­ing that only we have souls, or use tools, or are ca­pa­ble of self-aware­ness. Perhaps we should see what the birds have to say.An­i­mals have promi­nent speak­ing roles in many of our old­est sto­ries. Eve has a mem­o­rable con­ver­sa­tion with a snake. In Norse mythol­ogy, two ravens, Huginn and Muninn, serve as spies to the god Odin, whis­per­ing to him the news of the world. In many cul­tures, the language of birds” refers to a di­vine or per­fect lan­guage—the lan­guage of an­gels. In the sci­en­tific realm, how­ever, the no­tion that non­hu­man an­i­mals use lan­guage is of­ten seen as fool­ish or naïve. Some birds may be ex­cel­lent mim­ics, like par­rots, but they can also mimic chain­saws or bark­ing dogs; schol­ars don’t usu­ally con­sider im­i­ta­tion a form of un­der­stand­ing. The pre­vail­ing dogma is that birds sing ei­ther to im­press mates or to de­fend their ter­ri­tory. (I sus­pect that most of hu­man com­mu­ni­ca­tion could also be slot­ted into those cat­e­gories.) In col­lege, I was taught a stranger but sim­i­larly di­min­ish­ing idea: that song­birds sing in the morn­ing to burn fat, so that they are light enough to fly around dur­ing the day. Apparently, this idea is no longer taken se­ri­ously.Even among species we view as be­ing closer to our­selves, such as pri­mates, sci­en­tists have tended to talk about communication” in­stead of language.” But it’s tricky to say where the line is, or what we mean by communication,” since even bac­te­ria com­mu­ni­cate, as Berwick pointed out to me. I think it’s best to think of lan­guage not as speech but as a cog­ni­tive abil­ity in the mind that some­times leads to speech,” he said, giv­ing the ex­am­ple of in­ward con­ver­sa­tions we have with our­selves. The lin­guist Noam Chomsky has said, It’s about as likely that an ape will prove to have a lan­guage abil­ity as there is an is­land some­where with a species of flight­less birds wait­ing for hu­mans to teach them to fly.” Chomsky’s 2017 book on the evo­lu­tion of lan­guage, co-au­thored with Berwick, is ti­tled“Why Only Us.”Over the years, how­ever, some re­searchers have looked closely at the con­texts in which cer­tain an­i­mal vo­cal­iza­tions are made. In the late nine­teen-sev­en­ties, two pri­ma­tol­o­gists, Dorothy Cheney and Robert Seyfarth, were study­ing vervet mon­keys in Kenya. Vervets have dark faces and pale fur; they are about the size of a small back­pack and are hunted by pythons, ea­gles, and leop­ards. Cheney and Seyfarth doc­u­mented some­thing re­mark­able: one recorded vervet vo­cal­iza­tion made vervets look up, pre­sum­ably for ea­gles; an­other made them look down, pre­sum­ably for pythons; and a third sent them run­ning up into the trees, a good de­fense against ap­proach­ing leop­ards. Young vervets some­times use these calls fault­ily, per­haps sound­ing a leop­ard alarm for a warthog. But they get bet­ter as they grow up. They learn.A newer gen­er­a­tion of sci­en­tists has been try­ing to un­der­stand bird vo­cal­iza­tions. The alarm calls of Siberian jays can be said to have been par­tially trans­lated. One of their screeches in­di­cates a sit­ting hawk (which prompts other jays to come to­gether in a group), an­other a fly­ing hawk (jays hide, which makes them dif­fi­cult to spot), and a third a hawk ac­tively at­tack­ing (jays fly to the tree­tops to search for the at­tacker, and pos­si­bly flee). When cheery birds known as tufted tit­mice make a pierc­ing sound, other tit­mice may re­spond by col­lec­tively har­ry­ing an in­vad­ing preda­tor. Some birds even lie. Fork-tailed dron­gos—com­mon, in­nocu­ous-look­ing lit­tle dark birds that live in Africa—sometimes mimic the alarm calls of star­lings or meerkats. Duped lis­ten­ers flee the nonex­is­tent threat, leav­ing be­hind a buf­fet for the drongo.Upon see­ing an owl, a chick­adee might sound a loud chick-a-dee-dee-dee, adding dees in re­la­tion to how dan­ger­ous the preda­tor is per­ceived to be. This call is also un­der­stood by nuthatches, which will join in to mob and ha­rass the preda­tor, form­ing a kind of de­fen­sive al­liance. If you record an Australian bird warn­ing of a nearby cuckoo—cuck­oos leave their eggs in the nests of other species and of­ten kill their step-sib­lings—birds in China will un­der­stand the call.Klein­dor­fer con­sid­ers Cheney and Seyfarth, the pri­ma­tol­o­gists, to be im­por­tant sources of in­spi­ra­tion. After she moved to Australia, she and her col­leagues built up a sound li­brary of Australian song­birds. They also made record­ings of qui­eter, fa­mil­ial bird sounds, such as the in­cu­ba­tion calls. Each fam­ily unit, they dis­cov­ered, had its own familect,” a sys­tem of sounds that chicks learn from their par­ents. Curiously, chicks seemed to adopt sounds sung ei­ther by their mother or by their fa­ther—but they avoided the sounds used by both par­ents. If the mother sings ABCXYZ and the fa­ther sings ABCGHI, then the chicks tend to sing the sound units X, Y, Z, G, H, and I. It’s as if the young birds sep­a­rate them­selves from their par­ents by not speak­ing the shared sounds, but also stay close to their par­ents by learn­ing what’s unique to Mom and unique to Dad. When fe­male chicks grow up, they are at­tracted to mates whose reper­toire is fa­mil­iar (he’s one of us!), but not too fa­mil­iar (he’s not my brother or dad).Birds in gen­eral are turn­ing out to have in­tel­lec­tual abil­i­ties far greater than most peo­ple had imag­ined. It’s not just that par­rots and crows can do math as ably as young chil­dren, or that scrub jays clev­erly cache and then un­cache their food to fool other jays. Even in­con­spic­u­ous and un­cel­e­brated birds are ca­pa­ble of learn­ing, and of shar­ing their learn­ing with oth­ers. In the nine­teen-twen­ties, tits from Swaythling, England, fig­ured out how to open the caps of milk bot­tles, and by the late for­ties tits across Ireland, Wales, and England had learned the trick. If lan­guage is more a ca­pac­ity than it is a speech act, it seems pos­si­ble that birds pos­sess it.In 1889, Ludwig Paul Koch, an eight-year-old boy in Frankfurt, Germany, re­ceived a pre­sent from his fa­ther: an Edison phono­graph and some wax cylin­ders for record­ing sounds. The old­est known au­dio of bird­song is young Koch’s record­ing of his pet white-rumped shama, a small­ish song­bird with a dark head, an or­ange body, and feath­ers that re­sem­ble a white bus­tle on its glossy black tail. A shama sings like a small cham­ber or­ches­tra, with slip­pery, per­cus­sive, and sweet sounds in phrases of vary­ing lengths. Many sim­i­lar record­ings fol­lowed. In 1929, the Cornell Library of Natural Sounds—now the Macaulay Library—was started with a few hard-won record­ings of a spar­row, a wren, and a gros­beak. (Cornell is to or­nithol­ogy what the Juilliard School is to mu­sic.)Koch, who was Jewish, be­came a pro­fes­sional mu­si­cian but fled Germany in the nine­teen-thir­ties. In England, he be­came a beloved pres­ence on BBC ra­dio. Sounding like a singsong, san­guine sib­ling of Werner Herzog, he guided Brits through the charms of bird­song. (A yel­low icter­ine war­bler, he told lis­ten­ers, frequently called me by my Christian name . . . Ludwig, Ludwig.”) Koch of­ten ex­pressed the hope that such record­ings might be used for sci­ence. Many years later, they were.In 2010, Grant Van Horn was an un­der­grad­u­ate at U.C. San Diego and work­ing in a com­puter-vi­sion and ma­chine-learn­ing lab led by the com­puter sci­en­tist Serge Belongie. The lab was look­ing for a good data set to train an im­age-recog­ni­tion pro­gram. At the time, Van Horn told me, many of the top im­ages on Flickr, the pop­u­lar pho­tog­ra­phy Web site, were of birds. Van Horn was no birder, but he wanted to see if he and his col­leagues could teach a com­puter pro­gram to dis­tin­guish be­tween closely re­lated species, such as a house wren and a marsh wren. As it turned out, they could.The lab’s work soon at­tracted the at­ten­tion of or­nithol­ogy re­searchers at Cornell. Van Horn re­calls them telling him and his col­leagues, in the nicest pos­si­ble way, Look, guys—this data set is quaint and poorly con­structed, and the species that you chose to study make no sense. Do you want to ef­fec­tively redo this whole process, but do it in col­lab­o­ra­tion with us?’ ” When Van Horn vis­ited Cornell, the sci­en­tists took him out bird­ing every morn­ing and evening, and he re­mem­bers wish­ing that he could take their ex­per­tise back to California with him. The col­lab­o­ra­tion even­tu­ally helped the Cornell Lab of Ornithology de­velop Merlin Bird ID, an app that could re­li­ably iden­tify sev­eral hun­dred species of bird from pho­tographs. It proved im­mensely pop­u­lar—but the Cornell team had al­ways had larger am­bi­tions. They kept ask­ing, How can we do this with sound?’ ” Van Horn re­called. That was what the sci­en­tists were most in­ter­ested in. But he as­sumed that au­di­tory recog­ni­tion was out­side his ex­per­tise—un­til, out of cu­rios­ity, he at­tended a work­shop on au­dio-re­lated ma­chine learn­ing.“I kind of had an epiphany,” Van Horn said. Sound recog­ni­tion of­ten re­lied on spec­tro­grams, vi­sual rep­re­sen­ta­tions of sounds sim­i­lar to what you see in au­dio-edit­ing soft­ware. Mike Webster, an an­i­mal-com­mu­ni­ca­tion ex­pert at Cornell who di­rects the Macaulay Library, and who worked on Merlin, told me, When peo­ple fig­ured out how to vi­su­al­ize sound—how to ac­tu­ally take mea­sures of it—that led to just an ex­plo­sion of re­search and un­der­stand­ing about how and why birds com­mu­ni­cate with each other.” Much of the work in sound recog­ni­tion, Van Horn re­al­ized, was ac­tu­ally vi­sual: I thought, Let me bring these com­puter-vi­sion skills to bear.” An early test could dif­fer­en­ti­ate be­tween record­ings of alder fly­catch­ers and wil­low fly­catch­ers.“It’s still too heavy. What if in­stead of two arm­rests we just share one?”In 2021, the num­ber of bird record­ings in the Macaulay Library, many of which were sub­mit­ted by cit­i­zen sci­en­tists, reached a mil­lion. That same year, Cornell re­leased Merlin Sound ID, which was orig­i­nally trained on around two hun­dred and fifty hours of bird sounds, as well as on back­ground noises (whistling wind, pass­ing cars), all man­u­ally an­no­tated by ex­perts. At first, Sound ID could iden­tify about three hun­dred dif­fer­ent North American birds, with a bias to­ward those found around Cornell. Three years and a mil­lion ad­di­tional record­ings later, Sound ID can now very ac­cu­rately iden­tify about four­teen hun­dred species. The lab hopes that num­ber can grow to roughly eight thou­sand, out of around eleven thou­sand known bird species.Am­a­teurs now have a re­mark­able abil­ity to rec­og­nize the birds that are coo­ing or chirp­ing—which has gen­er­ated more in­ter­est in birds and di­rected more cit­i­zen-sci­ence record­ings to the Macaulay Library. But de­cod­ing the bird vo­cal­iza­tions is an­other mat­ter. One prob­lem is that cer­tain sorts of record­ings are more plen­ti­ful than oth­ers. Most of our data­base is songs,” Webster said. We can now un­der­stand songs at a level that we could­n’t be­fore.” Alarm calls are also rel­a­tively easy to cap­ture. But some­thing like nest chat­ter, which is qui­eter and less pre­dictable, is more elu­sive: There are whole cat­e­gories of bird com­mu­ni­ca­tion that we’ve hardly even started to look at.” Webster is­n’t ex­pect­ing there to be straight­for­ward trans­la­tions of birds’ sounds into hu­man lan­guage; an­i­mals live in per­cep­tual worlds that are just too dif­fer­ent from our own. Still, he sees ma­chine learn­ing as a pow­er­ful new tool. There are a lot of peo­ple who have dreams of us­ing A.I. to al­low us to de­ci­pher what an­i­mals are say­ing,” he told me.Af­ter three decades of re­search, Webster is prepar­ing to re­tire. When I asked him what he hoped the next gen­er­a­tion of sci­en­tists might learn, he thought for a mo­ment. Well, so­cial birds. They are con­stantly chat­ting to each other,” he said. Making lit­tle noises. Often very qui­etly. It’s like they’re hav­ing a whis­per con­ver­sa­tion. What in the hell are they say­ing to one an­other? I’d re­ally like to know.”Un­til my eleven-year-old daugh­ter be­came in­ter­ested in birds, I barely knew a star­ling from a spar­row. She once asked me, in­cred­u­lously, You’re say­ing you can’t tell a male spar­row from a fe­male?” For a long time, we lived just east of the Lincoln Tunnel, where birds” meant pi­geons and seag­ulls, but within weeks of mov­ing to Brooklyn we saw a red-tailed hawk on a lamp­post. My daugh­ter be­gan talk­ing about dark-eyed jun­cos and tufted tit­mice and pere­grine fal­cons; we started vis­it­ing bird sanc­tu­ar­ies, and I even­tu­ally out­grew my fa­voritism to­ward mam­mals. Like mil­lions of oth­ers, we started to use Merlin Bird ID. Usually, we heard birds be­fore we saw them. Some lo­cal spar­rows nest­ing in a hol­low pole on our block sounded like Laurel and Hardy bick­er­ing.“An­thro­po­mor­phism” is a fa­mil­iar term that de­scribes a com­mon er­ror: the as­sump­tion that an­i­mals have hu­man qual­i­ties. A less fa­mil­iar term, anthropectomy,” also de­scribes a kind of er­ror—that of base­lessly as­sum­ing an­i­mals don’t share cer­tain qual­i­ties with us. Which kind of er­ror is a per­son more likely to make? Or are these not er­rors but, rather, start­ing points, with some­one like Jane Goodall start­ing from the premise that 98.7 per cent of our DNA is shared with chimps, and some­one else start­ing from the fact that we hu­mans have se­quenced our own DNA and no other species has even in­vented pli­ers? Since we’re still ar­gu­ing about what lan­guage is, it’s dif­fi­cult to say which as­sump­tion about an­i­mal lan­guage is more pre­sump­tu­ous.Toshi­taka Suzuki first started to won­der if birds speak their own lan­guage dur­ing his last year of col­lege, at Toho University, in Tokyo. He was on a hike in the forests of Karuizawa when he wit­nessed what struck him as a strange drama among some com­mon Japanese tits, birds that re­sem­ble chick­adees. One tit called out dee-dee-dee near some scat­tered sun­flower seeds; other tits flew over and be­gan to eat. Then one bird called out hee-hee-hee, and the birds all flew off into nearby bushes,” Suzuki re­called. He could see no rea­son for them to aban­don their feast.Sec­onds later, a spar­row hawk swooped in; all of the tits had es­caped safely. I thought, Maybe hee-hee-hee means Hawk in­com­ing, run away!’ ” Suzuki said. He al­ready took birds se­ri­ously and knew a lot about them; he had stud­ied un­der Hiroshi Hasegawa, a sci­en­tist who was cen­tral in bring­ing the short-tailed al­ba­tross back from near-ex­tinc­tion. But Suzuki had thought of bird vo­cal­iza­tions as, for the most part, emo­tive, like mu­sic, or as a kind of beau­ti­ful non­sense. He has now de­voted eigh­teen years to re­search­ing tits and their com­mu­ni­ca­tion. I could­n’t have imag­ined how long I would be study­ing tits, be­cause I love other an­i­mals as well,” Suzuki told me. Like many re­searchers, he hopes that the more we un­der­stand birds the like­lier we are to pro­tect them.In April, 2023, at the University of Tokyo, Suzuki founded what he calls the world’s first lab­o­ra­tory specif­i­cally de­voted to an­i­mal lin­guis­tics. He ar­gues that more work should be done to ex­plore what cog­ni­tive abil­i­ties un­der­lie hu­man lan­guage—and then to in­ves­ti­gate whether these abil­i­ties are pre­sent in an­i­mals. (In many ways, this ap­proach mir­rors the work of Berwick and Chomsky, but leads to dif­fer­ent con­clu­sions.) Some are skep­ti­cal of his push to com­pare an­i­mal com­mu­ni­ca­tion to hu­man lan­guage. It’s just so far re­moved from the com­plex­ity of hu­man lan­guage that it does­n’t make sense to use the same word,” Todd Freeberg, an an­i­mal-com­mu­ni­ca­tion re­searcher at the University of Tennessee, told me. When a chick­adee am­pli­fies a call by adding dees, some re­searchers might say that they are en­gag­ing in ref­er­en­tial sig­nalling, by ad­just­ing their call to the se­ri­ous­ness of the threat. But Freeberg points out that ex­tra dees could also be a re­sult of height­ened arousal, in gen­eral—less a con­scious mes­sage than a phys­i­cal re­sponse.Like Kleindorfer, Suzuki took an in­ter­est in nests. Early on, he showed that chicks in nest­ing boxes re­spond to a call as­so­ci­ated with crow sight­ings by crouch­ing, and to a call as­so­ci­ated with snakes by flee­ing the nest al­to­gether. The arc of Suzuki’s re­search has, to some ex­tent, fol­lowed a se­ries of ar­gu­ments about what qual­i­ties are re­quired for com­mu­ni­ca­tion to rise to the sta­tus of lan­guage. Humans are noted for their abil­ity to form a men­tal im­age—a con­cept—of what they are com­mu­ni­cat­ing. Suzuki de­signed an ex­per­i­ment in which he played a va­ri­ety of calls and moved a stick in a va­ri­ety of ways; only when he played a snake-alarm call and moved a stick in a snake­like way did the birds tend to re­act as if a snake were pre­sent. To him, this sug­gested that they had some con­cept of snake-ness. (He said that the ex­per­i­ment was in­spired by the way that hu­mans per­ceive shapes in clouds.)In a 2023 study, Suzuki showed that tits re­sponded dif­fer­ently to a recorded ABCD call than they did to a remixed ver­sion of the call, such as DABC—a po­ten­tial chal­lenge to lin­guists who see so­phis­ti­cated syn­tax as be­ing unique to hu­man lan­guage. (Studies of south­ern pied bab­blers and of chest­nut-crowned bab­blers also have in­ter­est­ing syn­tax re­sults.) Symbolic ges­tures—also of­ten con­sid­ered unique to hu­mans—were ad­dressed in a par­tic­u­larly adorable Suzuki pa­per, in 2024. His team watched mated pairs of tits as they en­tered their nest boxes. The open­ing to each nest box was small, al­low­ing only one bird at a time to pass. But some­times one bird, usu­ally the fe­male, flut­tered its wings in what seemed to be an after you” ges­ture. The other bird would then en­ter the box first. The flut­ter­ing did­n’t point at the nest box. In Suzuki’s view, this sug­gested that the flut­ter was not a sim­ple in­di­ca­tion but, in­stead, a sym­bolic ges­ture—an­other item crossed off on the unique-to-hu­man-lan­guage list.Per­haps the nest-box study needs to be repli­cated; per­haps there are al­ter­na­tive in­ter­pre­ta­tions of the re­sults in the con­cept and syn­tax stud­ies. Suzuki is open to such cri­tiques. But he is also skep­ti­cal of many promi­nent ideas in lin­guis­tics, such as Chomsky and Berwick’s ar­gu­ment that a slight evo­lu­tion­ary change in the brain un­locked a new lin­guis­tic ca­pac­ity in hu­mans: the unique and pow­er­ful abil­ity to con­nect in­di­vid­ual units in a hi­er­ar­chi­cal and ex­pres­sive way. (Suzuki thinks that lan­guage more likely emerged bit by bit.) By Suzuki’s lat­est count, the tit’s vo­cal reper­toire has more than two hun­dred dis­tinct calls and phrases. He has many more ex­per­i­ments to con­duct.Re­cently, my daugh­ter and I took an early-morn­ing trip to Little Stony Point, in the Hudson Valley, and met up with two peo­ple who have no par­tic­u­lar need for an app like Merlin Bird ID. Andrew C. Vallely does field-or­nithol­ogy work for the American Museum of Natural History; he’s be­come friends, by way of bird-watch­ing, with Jeffrey Yang, an ed­i­tor at New Directions Publishing. Yang had been see­ing a lot of mi­grat­ing war­blers, which had flown well over a thou­sand miles—did we want to come try our luck? At his sug­ges­tion, I warned my daugh­ter that there was no telling whether we’d ac­tu­ally see any.About five min­utes down the trail, in a not par­tic­u­larly dis­tin­guished wood (we could still hear cars and an ex­ca­va­tor across the river), we saw a king­fisher div­ing and an ado­les­cent ea­gle on a bare tree. As we walked, stopped, walked, stopped, we re­peat­edly heard what sounded like the call of a red-tailed hawk. But it soon be­came clear, at least to Vallely and Yang, that it was a jay mim­ic­k­ing a hawk. They do that some­times,” Vallely told me.“That’s one thought,” he said. Vocal mim­icry can be pretty mys­te­ri­ous.”We heard the tea ket­tle tea ket­tle” call of a Carolina wren; it sounded like a game of mar­bles to me. We saw a war­bling vireo, a Cape May war­bler, a black­poll war­bler, and a black-and-white war­bler—birds so small that it was dif­fi­cult to fathom how far some of them had trav­elled to be there. We heard lit­tle chips that sounded like a win­dow be­ing cleaned; a crick­ety de­crescendo that was not made by crick­ets; a sound like a trill run­ning into a wall; a high-pitched three-fast-one-slow, like a child play­ing Beethoven’s Fifth Symphony. We en­coun­tered forty-four species by Yang’s able count, and at the very end we saw a Swainson’s thrush, who ap­par­ently was­n’t in the mood to show off. Bird-watching, I thought, is a mis­lead­ing term. So much of the fleet­ing, pre­sent-tense plea­sure of it is bird-lis­ten­ing.The quiet of the pan­demic brought nat­ural sounds to the fore­ground for Maddie Cusimano, who was then a grad­u­ate re­searcher of au­di­tory per­cep­tion at M.I.T.’s Center for Brains, Minds, and Machines. Like a lot of peo­ple, I had the sense of get­ting to know the birds around me for the first time,” she told me. Two doves were of­ten vis­i­ble from her win­dow; she read that, in some dove pairs, one bird sings to the other in the morn­ings, and in the evenings the roles re­verse. Cusimano was fa­mil­iar enough with ma­chine learn­ing that, when she tried out bird-iden­ti­fi­ca­tion apps, she thought, I could help make this kind of thing.Cusi­mano is now a se­nior sci­en­tist spe­cial­iz­ing in A.I. re­search at the Earth Species Project (E.S.P.), a non­profit ded­i­cated to using ar­ti­fi­cial in­tel­li­gence to de­code non-hu­man com­mu­ni­ca­tion.” E.S.P.’s cur­rent ef­forts ex­am­ine such species as ze­bra finches, crows, and bel­uga whales, but its early work has been pre­oc­cu­pied with pre­lim­i­nary chal­lenges: the cocktail-party prob­lem” of pick­ing up in­di­vid­ual sounds in a noisy en­vi­ron­ment; how to cor­re­late par­tic­u­lar noises with the pre­cise con­texts in which they oc­cur. It’s like we want to write the Magna Carta, but first we need to make the quill,” Katie Zacarian, the or­ga­ni­za­tion’s co-founder and C.E.O., told me. Zacarian is­n’t ex­pect­ing a Google Translate for an­i­mal lan­guages, but she does be­lieve that we can un­der­stand an­i­mals bet­ter. She re­mem­bers that, as a kid, peo­ple of­ten brought her fa­ther, an en­to­mol­o­gist, pic­tures and spec­i­mens and asked: What is this? Her mother was a re­searcher and an ad­min­is­tra­tor of mul­ti­lin­gual ed­u­ca­tion pro­grams. There’s this un­der­ly­ing cur­rent, in their work, of de­cod­ing,” she told me.When I talked to Cusimano, on Zoom, she pulled up a col­lec­tion of sound files of crows. Her data set comes from Daniela Canestrari and Vittorio Baglione, re­searchers at the University of León, in Spain, who have been study­ing Spanish car­rion crows for more than twenty-five years. Cusimano has spent count­less hours sit­ting in San Francisco, lis­ten­ing to these birds, and can some­times guess which one she’s hear­ing. This is maybe what you ex­pect a crow to sound like,” she said, play­ing me two caws. But then there’s also this,” she said, play­ing a whis­pery rasp. Some sounds are very long.” She played a ghostly oooo. Then these two sounds, which you would never think were com­ing from crows.” One sounded like the click of a com­puter mouse; an­other sounded froggy. Her fa­vorite record­ing re­minded me of a duck’s quack. I love these sounds,” she told me.One am­bi­tion of Cusimano’s work is to find cor­re­la­tions be­tween these var­ied vo­cal­iza­tions and the pre­cise con­texts in which they oc­curred. Research part­ners re­cently iden­ti­fied a quiet grunt that is most of­ten made right when an adult crow re­turns to a nest—per­haps a way of say­ing, Wake up!” A small bio-log­ger on the back of a crow can pro­vide au­dio along with other data—a bird’s-tail view. You hear their wing­beats, you can hear their friends call­ing, and them call­ing back to their friends,” Cusimano told me. The data feels in­ti­mate: You hear baby chicks a dis­tance away, then you hear the bird take off, and the chick sounds are get­ting louder. And then the crow lands in the nest. You’re in the mid­dle of this crow fam­ily.”Can a ma­chine be trained to dis­tin­guish in­di­vid­ual birds by the sounds they make? Can it pick up on vo­cal­iza­tions across in­di­vid­u­als which share sim­i­lar func­tions? Machine learn­ing is ex­cel­lent at de­tect­ing cor­re­la­tions, but some are ir­rel­e­vant and even mis­lead­ing. Cusimano de­vel­oped an al­go­rithm to dis­tin­guish among caws made by var­i­ous crows, which had names such as Naranja, Rosa, and Azul. She seemed to have suc­ceeded. Then she re­al­ized that the com­puter might be cat­e­go­riz­ing the sounds based on dis­tinc­tive back­ground noises, which cor­re­sponded to the place­ment of the record­ing de­vices. The al­go­rithms can pick up on tiny lit­tle clues that con­found the ac­tual prob­lems we want to find an­swers to,” she said.Those who live and work along­side an­i­mals, whether they’re sci­en­tists or not, of­ten think, as a mat­ter of course, that an­i­mals can speak with one an­other, and in depth. Instead of be­ing sur­prised by the dis­cov­ery of each unexpected” an­i­mal abil­ity, maybe we should be sur­prised that hu­mans have such low ex­pec­ta­tions. Many of us laugh—or shake our heads sadly—when we read that Descartes sup­pos­edly threw a cat out of a win­dow to see if it would show fear, as a sort of test for con­scious­ness. (He be­lieved that non­hu­man an­i­mals were sense­less au­toma­tons.) Yet many of us would also con­sider it a won­der that, ac­cord­ing to a re­cent study, ele­phants seem to have dis­tinct names for one an­other, which their ele­phant friends and fam­ily use among them­selves.When I started re­search­ing this story, I was amazed by each ad­di­tional avian ac­com­plish­ment that I learned about, es­pe­cially in small, or­di­nary birds. It was­n’t only that they com­mu­ni­cated this or that to one an­other but that they were full of con­cerns—that they were at the cen­ter of their own worlds. But should­n’t I have in­tu­ited that this was the case all along? I had base­lessly as­sumed that birds had lit­tle on their minds. The other day, my daugh­ter and I were walk­ing to her soc­cer prac­tice, pass­ing by spar­rows and also peo­ple. We know al­most noth­ing about birds,” she told me. There’s so much we don’t even no­tice.” She thought for a mo­ment. I think they have just as much lan­guage as we do, but a lot of it is in their mind. So we don’t hear it.” ♦

...

Read the original on www.newyorker.com »

To add this web app to your iOS home screen tap the share button and select "Add to the Home Screen".

10HN is also available as an iOS App

If you visit 10HN only rarely, check out the the best articles from the past week.

If you like 10HN please leave feedback and share

Visit pancik.com for more.